[Vision2020] The Science of Why We Don't Believe Science

Joe Campbell philosopher.joe at gmail.com
Wed Jun 19 09:20:03 PDT 2013


Wow fantastic article! There seems to be an easy fix: base policy on actual
science. Why? Science has developed ways of weeding out bias, not
completely perhaps but as much as possible. Individuals and political
interest groups do not. Joe


On Wed, Jun 19, 2013 at 8:51 AM, Art Deco <art.deco.studios at gmail.com>wrote:

> Forward this to the Christ Church Cult and the millions of other religious
> crackpots.  Don't count on the con artists among them to help out though.
> Religious belief, impenetrable to rational though, is big, big business.
>
> w.
>
>
> On Wed, Jun 19, 2013 at 11:32 AM, Ron Force <rforce2003 at yahoo.com> wrote:
>
>> ** The Science of Why We Don’t Believe ScienceHow our brains fool us on
>> climate, creationism, and the vaccine-autism link. **
>> *By **Chris Mooney* <https://twitter.com/chriscmooney>
>> *“A MAN WITH A CONVICTION *is a hard man to change. Tell him you
>> disagree and he turns away. Show him facts or figures and he questions your
>> sources. Appeal to logic and he fails to see your point.” So wrote the
>> celebrated Stanford University psychologist Leon Festinger, in a passage
>> that might have been referring to climate change denial—the persistent
>> rejection, on the part of so many Americans today, of what we know about
>> global warming and its human causes. But it was too early for that—this was
>> the 1950s—and Festinger was actually describing a famous case study<http://www.powells.com/biblio/61-9781617202803-1> in
>> psychology.
>> Festinger and several of his colleagues had infiltrated the Seekers, a
>> small Chicago-area cult whose members thought they were communicating with
>> aliens—including one, “Sananda,” who they believed was the astral
>> incarnation of Jesus Christ. The group was led by Dorothy Martin, a
>> Dianetics devotee who transcribed the interstellar messages through
>> automatic writing.
>> Through her, the aliens had given the precise date of an Earth-rending
>> cataclysm: December 21, 1954. Some of Martin’s followers quit their jobs
>> and sold their property, expecting to be rescued by a flying saucer when
>> the continent split asunder and a new sea swallowed much of the United
>> States. The disciples even went so far as to remove brassieres and rip
>> zippers out of their trousers—the metal, they believed, would pose a danger
>> on the spacecraft.
>> Festinger and his team were with the cult when the prophecy failed.
>> First, the “boys upstairs” (as the aliens were sometimes called) did not
>> show up and rescue the Seekers. Then December 21 arrived without incident.
>> It was the moment Festinger had been waiting for: How would people so
>> emotionally invested in a belief system react, now that it had been soundly
>> refuted?
>> At first, the group struggled for an explanation. But then
>> rationalization set in. A new message arrived, announcing that they’d all
>> been spared at the last minute. Festinger summarized the extraterrestrials’
>> new pronouncement: “The little group, sitting all night long, had spread so
>> much light that God had saved the world from destruction.” Their
>> willingness to believe in the prophecy had saved Earth from the prophecy!
>> From that day forward, the Seekers, previously shy of the press and
>> indifferent toward evangelizing, began to proselytize. “Their sense of
>> urgency was enormous,” wrote Festinger. The devastation of all they had
>> believed had made them even more certain of their beliefs.
>> *IN THE ANNALS OF DENIAL,* it doesn’t get much more extreme than the
>> Seekers. They lost their jobs, the press mocked them, and there were
>> efforts to keep them away from impressionable young minds. But while
>> Martin’s space cult might lie at on the far end of the spectrum of human
>> self-delusion, there’s plenty to go around. And since Festinger’s day, an
>> array of new discoveries in psychology and neuroscience has further
>> demonstrated how our preexisting beliefs, far more than any new facts, can
>> skew our thoughts and even color what we consider our most dispassionate
>> and logical conclusions. This tendency toward so-called “motivated
>> reasoning <http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2270237>” helps explain
>> why we find groups so polarized over matters where the evidence is so
>> unequivocal: climate change, vaccines, “death panels,” the birthplace and religion
>> of the president <http://www-personal.umich.edu/~bnyhan/obama-muslim.pdf> (PDF),
>> and much else. It would seem that expecting people to be convinced by the
>> facts flies in the face of, you know, the facts.
>> The theory of motivated reasoning builds on a key insight of modern
>> neuroscience <https://motherjones.com/files/descartes.pdf> (PDF):
>> Reasoning is actually suffused with emotion (or what researchers often call
>> “affect”). Not only are the two inseparable, but our positive or negative
>> feelings about people, things, and ideas arise much more rapidly than our
>> conscious thoughts, in a matter of milliseconds—fast enough to detect with
>> an EEG device, but long before we’re aware of it. That shouldn’t be
>> surprising: Evolution required us to react very quickly to stimuli in our
>> environment. It’s a “basic human survival skill,” explains political
>> scientist Arthur Lupia <http://www-personal.umich.edu/~lupia/> of the
>> University of Michigan. We push threatening information away; we pull
>> friendly information close. We apply fight-or-flight reflexes not only to
>> predators, but to data itself.
>>
>> We apply fight-or-flight reflexes not only to predators, but to data
>> itself.
>>
>> Consider a person who has heard about a scientific discovery that deeply
>> challenges her belief in divine creation—a new hominid, say, that confirms
>> our evolutionary origins. What happens next, explains political scientist Charles
>> Taber <http://www.stonybrook.edu/polsci/ctaber/> of Stony Brook
>> University, is a subconscious negative response to the new information—and
>> that response, in turn, guides the type of memories and associations formed
>> in the conscious mind. “They retrieve thoughts that are consistent with
>> their previous beliefs,” says Taber, “and that will lead them to build an
>> argument and challenge what they’re hearing.”
>> In other words, when we think we’re reasoning, we may instead be
>> rationalizing. Or to use an analogy offered by University of Virginia
>> psychologist Jonathan Haidt <http://people.virginia.edu/~jdh6n/>: We may
>> think we’re being scientists, butwe’re actually being lawyers<https://motherjones.com/files/emotional_dog_and_rational_tail.pdf> (PDF).
>> Our “reasoning” is a means to a predetermined end—winning our “case”—and is
>> shot through with biases. They include “confirmation bias,” in which we
>> give greater heed to evidence and arguments that bolster our beliefs, and
>> “disconfirmation bias,” in which we expend disproportionate energy trying
>> to debunk or refute views and arguments that we find uncongenial.
>> That’s a lot of jargon, but we all understand these mechanisms when it
>> comes to interpersonal relationships. If I don’t want to believe that my
>> spouse is being unfaithful, or that my child is a bully, I can go to great
>> lengths to explain away behavior that seems obvious to everybody
>> else—everybody who isn’t too emotionally invested to accept it, anyway.
>> That’s not to suggest that we aren’t also motivated to perceive the world
>> accurately—we are. Or that we never change our minds—we do. It’s just that
>> we have other important goals besides accuracy—including identity
>> affirmation and protecting one’s sense of self—and often those make us
>> highly resistant to changing our beliefs when the facts say we should.
>>
>> Scientific evidence is highly susceptible to misinterpretation. Giving
>> ideologues scientific data that’s relevant to their beliefs is like
>> unleashing them in the motivated-reasoning equivalent of a candy store.
>>
>> *MODERN SCIENCE* *ORIGINATED* from an attempt to weed out such
>> subjective lapses—what that great 17th century theorist of the scientific
>> method, Francis Bacon, dubbed the “idols of the mind.” Even if individual
>> researchers are prone to falling in love with their own theories, the
>> broader processes of peer review and institutionalized skepticism are
>> designed to ensure that, eventually, the best ideas prevail.
>> Our individual responses to the conclusions that science reaches,
>> however, are quite another matter. Ironically, in part because researchers
>> employ so much nuance and strive to disclose all remaining sources of
>> uncertainty, scientific evidence is highly susceptible to selective reading
>> and misinterpretation. Giving ideologues or partisans scientific data
>> that’s relevant to their beliefs is like unleashing them in the
>> motivated-reasoning equivalent of a candy store.
>> Sure enough, a large number of psychological studies have shown that
>> people respond to scientific or technical evidence in ways that justify
>> their preexisting beliefs. In a classic 1979 experiment<http://synapse.princeton.edu/~sam/lord_ross_lepper79_JPSP_biased-assimilation-and-attitude-polarization.pdf> (PDF),
>> pro- and anti-death penalty advocates were exposed to descriptions of two
>> fake scientific studies: one supporting and one undermining the notion that
>> capital punishment deters violent crime and, in particular, murder. They
>> were also shown detailed methodological critiques of the fake studies—and
>> in a scientific sense, neither study was stronger than the other. Yet in
>> each case, advocates more heavily criticized the study whose conclusions
>> disagreed with their own, while describing the study that was more
>> ideologically congenial as more “convincing.”
>> Since then, similar results have been found for how people respond to
>> “evidence” about affirmative action, gun control, the accuracy of gay
>> stereotypes <http://psp.sagepub.com/content/23/6/636.abstract>, and much
>> else. Even when study subjects are explicitly instructed to be unbiased and
>> even-handed about the evidence, they often fail.
>> And it’s not just that people twist or selectively read scientific
>> evidence to support their preexisting views. According to research by Yale
>> Law School professor Dan Kahan<http://www.law.yale.edu/faculty/DKahan.htm> and
>> his colleagues, people’s deep-seated views about morality, and about the
>> way society should be ordered, strongly predict whom they consider to be a
>> legitimate scientific expert in the first place—and thus where they
>> consider “scientific consensus” to lie on contested issues.
>> In Kahan’s research<https://motherjones.com/files/kahan_paper_cultural_cognition_of_scientific_consesus.pdf> (PDF),
>> individuals are classified, based on their cultural values, as either
>> “individualists” or “communitarians,” and as either “hierarchical” or
>> “egalitarian” in outlook. (Somewhat oversimplifying, you can think of
>> hierarchical individualists as akin to conservative Republicans, and
>> egalitarian communitarians as liberal Democrats.) In one study, subjects in
>> the different groups were asked to help a close friend determine the risks
>> associated with climate change, sequestering nuclear waste, or concealed
>> carry laws: “The friend tells you that he or she is planning to read a book
>> about the issue but would like to get your opinion on whether the author
>> seems like a knowledgeable and trustworthy expert.” A subject was then
>> presented with the résumé of a fake expert “depicted as a member of the
>> National Academy of Sciences who had earned a Ph.D. in a pertinent field
>> from one elite university and who was now on the faculty of another.” The
>> subject was then shown a book excerpt by that “expert,” in which the risk
>> of the issue at hand was portrayed as high or low, well-founded or
>> speculative. The results were stark: When the scientist’s position stated
>> that global warming is real and human-caused, for instance, only 23 percent
>> of hierarchical individualists agreed the person was a “trustworthy and
>> knowledgeable expert.” Yet 88 percent of egalitarian communitarians
>> accepted the same scientist’s expertise. Similar divides were observed on
>> whether nuclear waste can be safely stored underground and whether letting
>> people carry guns deters crime. (The alliances did not always hold. In another
>> study<http://digitalcommons.law.yale.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1095&context=fss_papers> (PDF),
>> hierarchs and communitarians were in favor of laws that would compel the
>> mentally ill to accept treatment, whereas individualists and egalitarians
>> were opposed.)
>>
>> Head-on attempts to persuade can sometimes trigger a backfire effect,
>> where people not only fail to change their minds when confronted with the
>> facts—they may hold their wrong views more tenaciously than ever.
>>
>> In other words, people rejected the validity of a scientific source
>> because its conclusion contradicted their deeply held views—and thus the
>> relative risks inherent in each scenario. A hierarchal individualist finds
>> it difficult to believe that the things he prizes (commerce, industry, a
>> man’s freedom to possess a gun to defend his family<http://digitalcommons.law.yale.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1095&context=fss_papers>)
>> (PDF) could lead to outcomes deleterious to society. Whereas egalitarian
>> communitarians tend to think that the free market causes harm, that
>> patriarchal families mess up kids, and that people can’t handle their guns.
>> The study subjects weren’t “anti-science”—not in their own minds, anyway.
>> It’s just that “science” was whatever they wanted it to be. “We’ve come to
>> a misadventure, a bad situation where diverse citizens, who rely on diverse
>> systems of cultural certification, are in conflict,” says Kahan<http://seagrant.oregonstate.edu/blogs/communicatingclimate/transcripts/Episode_10b_Dan_Kahan.html>
>> .
>> And that undercuts the standard notion that the way to persuade people is
>> via evidence and argument. In fact, head-on attempts to persuade can
>> sometimes trigger a backfire effect, where people not only fail to change
>> their minds when confronted with the facts—they may hold their wrong views
>> more tenaciously than ever.
>> Take, for instance, the question of whether Saddam Hussein possessed
>> hidden weapons of mass destruction just before the US invasion of Iraq in
>> 2003. When political scientists Brendan Nyhan and Jason Reiflershowed
>> subjects fake newspaper articles<http://www-personal.umich.edu/~bnyhan/nyhan-reifler.pdf> (PDF)
>> in which this was first suggested (in a 2004 quote from President Bush) and
>> then refuted (with the findings of the Bush-commissioned Iraq Survey Group
>> report, which found no evidence of active WMD programs in pre-invasion
>> Iraq), they found that conservatives were more likely than before to
>> believe the claim. (The researchers also tested how liberals responded when
>> shown that Bush did not actually “ban” embryonic stem-cell research.
>> Liberals weren’t particularly amenable to persuasion, either, but no
>> backfire effect was observed.)
>> Another study gives some inkling of what may be going through people’s
>> minds when they resist persuasion. Northwestern University sociologist Monica
>> Prasad <http://www.sociology.northwestern.edu/faculty/prasad/home.html> and
>> her colleagues wanted to test whether they could dislodge the notion that
>> Saddam Hussein and Al Qaeda were secretly collaborating among those most
>> likely to believe it—Republican partisans from highly GOP-friendly
>> counties. So the researchers set up a study<http://sociology.buffalo.edu/documents/hoffmansocinquiryarticle_000.pdf> (PDF)
>> in which they discussed the topic with some of these Republicans in person.
>> They would cite the findings of the 9/11 Commission, as well as a statement
>> in which George W. Bush himself denied his administration had “said the
>> 9/11 attacks were orchestrated between Saddam and Al Qaeda.”
>>
>> One study showed that not even Bush’s own words could change the minds of
>> Bush voters who believed there was an Iraq-Al Qaeda link.
>>
>> As it turned out, not even Bush’s own words could change the minds of
>> these Bush voters—just 1 of the 49 partisans who originally believed the
>> Iraq-Al Qaeda claim changed his or her mind. Far more common was resisting
>> the correction in a variety of ways, either by coming up with
>> counterarguments or by simply being unmovable:
>> *Interviewer: *[T]he September 11 Commission found no link between
>> Saddam and 9/11, and this is what President Bush said. Do you have any
>> comments on either of those?
>> *Respondent: *Well, I bet they say that the Commission didn’t have any
>> proof of it but I guess we still can have our opinions and feel that way
>> even though they say that.
>> The same types of responses are already being documented on divisive
>> topics facing the current administration. Take the “Ground Zero mosque.”
>> Using information from the political myth-busting siteFactCheck.org<http://www.factcheck.org/>,
>> a team at Ohio State presented subjects<http://www.comm.ohio-state.edu/kgarrett/FactcheckMosqueRumors.pdf> (PDF)
>> with a detailed rebuttal to the claim that “Feisal Abdul Rauf, the Imam
>> backing the proposed Islamic cultural center and mosque, is a
>> terrorist-sympathizer.” Yet among those who were aware of the rumor and
>> believed it, fewer than a third changed their minds.
>> A key question—and one that’s difficult to answer—is how “irrational” all
>> this is. On the one hand, it doesn’t make sense to discard an entire belief
>> system, built up over a lifetime, because of some new snippet of
>> information. “It is quite possible to say, ‘I reached this
>> pro-capital-punishment decision based on real information that I arrived at
>> over my life,’” explains Stanford social psychologist Jon Krosnick<http://communication.stanford.edu/faculty/krosnick/>.
>> Indeed, there’s a sense in which science denial could be considered keenly
>> “rational.” In certain conservative communities, explains Yale’s Kahan,
>> “People who say, ‘I think there’s something to climate change,’ that’s
>> going to mark them out as a certain kind of person, and their life is going
>> to go less well.”
>> This may help explain a curious pattern Nyhan and his colleagues found
>> when they tried to test the fallacy<http://www-personal.umich.edu/~bnyhan/obama-muslim.pdf> (PDF)
>> that President Obama is a Muslim. When a nonwhite researcher was
>> administering their study, research subjects were amenable to changing
>> their minds about the president’s religion and updating incorrect views.
>> But when only white researchers were present, GOP survey subjects in
>> particular were more likely to believe the Obama Muslim myth than before.
>> The subjects were using “social desirabililty” to tailor their beliefs (or
>> stated beliefs, anyway) to whoever was listening.
>>
>> A predictor of whether you accept the science of global warming? Whether
>> you’re a Republican or a Democrat.
>>
>> Which leads us to the media. When people grow polarized over a body of
>> evidence, or a resolvable matter of fact, the cause may be some form of
>> biased reasoning, but they could also be receiving skewed information to
>> begin with—or a complicated combination of both. In the Ground Zero mosque
>> case, for instance, a follow-up study<http://www.comm.ohio-state.edu/kgarrett/MediaMosqueRumors.pdf> (PDF)
>> showed that survey respondents who watched Fox News were more likely to
>> believe the Rauf rumor and three related ones—and they believed them more
>> strongly than non-Fox watchers.
>> Okay, so people gravitate toward information that confirms what they
>> believe, and they select sources that deliver it. Same as it ever was,
>> right? Maybe, but the problem is arguably growing more acute, given the way
>> we now consume information—through the Facebook links of friends, or tweets
>> that lack nuance or context, or “narrowcast<http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Narrowcasting>”
>> and often highly ideological media that have relatively small, like-minded
>> audiences. Those basic human survival skills of ours, says Michigan’s
>> Arthur Lupia, are “not well-adapted to our information age.”
>> *IF YOU WANTED TO SHOW *how and why fact is ditched in favor of
>> motivated reasoning, you could find no better test case than climate
>> change. After all, it’s an issue where you have highly technical
>> information on one hand and very strong beliefs on the other. And sure
>> enough, one key predictor of whether you accept the science of global
>> warming is whether you’re a Republican or a Democrat. The two groups have
>> been growing more divided in their views about the topic, even as the
>> science becomes more unequivocal.
>> So perhaps it should come as no surprise that more education doesn’t
>> budge Republican views. On the contrary: In a 2008 Pew survey<http://people-press.org/report/417/a-deeper-partisan-divide-over-global-warming>,
>> for instance, only 19 percent of college-educated Republicans agreed that
>> the planet is warming due to human actions, versus 31 percent of
>> non-college educated Republicans. In other words, a higher education
>> correlated with an increased likelihood of denying the science on the
>> issue. Meanwhile, among Democrats and independents, more education
>> correlated with greater acceptance of the science.
>> Other studies have shown a similar effect: Republicans who think they
>> understand the global warming issue best are least concerned about it; and
>> among Republicans and those with higher levels of distrust of science in
>> general, learning more about the issue doesn’t increase one’s concern about
>> it. What’s going on here? Well, according to Charles Taber and Milton Lodge
>> of Stony Brook, one insidious aspect of motivated reasoning is that
>> political sophisticates are prone to be more biased than those who know
>> less about the issues. “People who have a dislike of some policy—for
>> example, abortion—if they’re unsophisticated they can just reject it out of
>> hand,” says Lodge. “But if they’re sophisticated, they can go one step
>> further and start coming up with counterarguments.” These individuals are
>> just as emotionally driven and biased as the rest of us, but they’re able
>> to generate more and better reasons to explain why they’re right—and so
>> their minds become harder to change.
>> That may be why the selectively quoted emails of Climategate were so
>> quickly and easily seized upon by partisans as evidence of scandal.
>> Cherry-picking is precisely the sort of behavior you would expect motivated
>> reasoners to engage in to bolster their views—and whatever you may think
>> about Climategate, the emails were a rich trove of new information upon
>> which to impose one’s ideology.
>> Climategate had a substantial impact on public opinion, according toAnthony
>> Leiserowitz <http://environment.yale.edu/profile/leiserowitz/>, director
>> of the Yale Project on Climate Change Communication<http://environment.yale.edu/climate/>.
>> It contributed to an overall drop in public concern about climate change
>> and a significant loss of trust in scientists. But—as we should expect by
>> now—these declines were concentrated among particular groups of Americans:
>> Republicans, conservatives, and those with “individualistic” values.
>> Liberals and those with “egalitarian” values didn’t lose much trust in
>> climate science or scientists at all. “In some ways, Climategate was like a
>> Rorschach test,” Leiserowitz says, “with different groups interpreting
>> ambiguous facts in very different ways.”
>>
>> Is there a case study of science denial that largely occupies the
>> political left? Yes: the claim that childhood vaccines are causing an
>> epidemic of autism.
>>
>> So is there a case study of science denial that largely occupies the
>> political left? Yes: the claim that childhood vaccines are causing an
>> epidemic of autism. Its most famous proponents are an environmentalist (Robert
>> F. Kennedy Jr<http://www.huffingtonpost.com/robert-f-kennedy-jr-and-david-kirby/vaccine-court-autism-deba_b_169673.html>.)
>> and numerous Hollywood celebrities (most notably Jenny McCarthy<http://www.huffingtonpost.com/jenny-mccarthy/vaccine-autism-debate_b_806857.html> and
>> Jim Carrey). The*Huffington Post *gives a very large megaphone to
>> denialists. And Seth Mnookin <http://sethmnookin.com/>, author of the
>> new book *The Panic Virus*<http://www.powells.com/biblio/1-9781439158647-0>,
>> notes that if you want to find vaccine deniers, all you need to do is go
>> hang out at Whole Foods.
>> Vaccine denial has all the hallmarks of a belief system that’s not
>> amenable to refutation. Over the past decade, the assertion that childhood
>> vaccines are driving autism rates has been undermined<http://discovermagazine.com/2009/jun/06-why-does-vaccine-autism-controversy-live-on/article_print> by
>> multiple epidemiological studies—as well as the simple fact that autism
>> rates continue to rise, even though the alleged offending agent in vaccines
>> (a mercury-based preservative called thimerosal) has long since been
>> removed.
>> Yet the true believers persist—critiquing each new study that challenges
>> their views, and even rallying to the defense of vaccine-autism researcher
>> Andrew Wakefield, after his 1998 *Lancet *paper<http://www.thelancet.com/journals/lancet/article/PIIS0140673697110960/fulltext>—which
>> originated the current vaccine scare—was retracted and he subsequently lost
>> his license<http://www.gmc-uk.org/Wakefield_SPM_and_SANCTION.pdf_32595267.pdf> (PDF)
>> to practice medicine. But then, why should we be surprised? Vaccine deniers
>> created their own partisan media, such as the website Age of Autism, that
>> instantly blast out critiques and counterarguments whenever any new
>> development casts further doubt on anti-vaccine views.
>> It all raises the question: Do left and right differ in any meaningful
>> way when it comes to biases in processing information, or are we all
>> equally susceptible?
>> There are some clear differences. Science denial today is considerably
>> more prominent on the political right—once you survey climate and related
>> environmental issues, anti-evolutionism, attacks on reproductive health
>> science by the Christian right, and stem-cell and biomedical matters. More
>> tellingly, anti-vaccine positions are virtually nonexistent among
>> Democratic officeholders today—whereas anti-climate-science views are
>> becoming monolithic among Republican elected officials.
>> Some researchers have suggested that there are psychological differences
>> between the left and the right that might impact responses to new
>> information—that conservatives are more rigid and authoritarian, and
>> liberals more tolerant of ambiguity. Psychologist John Jost of New York
>> University has further argued that conservatives are “system justifiers”:
>> They engage in motivated reasoning to defend the status quo.
>>
>> We all have blinders in some situations. The question then becomes: What
>> can be done to counteract human nature?
>>
>> This is a contested area, however, because as soon as one tries to
>> psychoanalyze inherent political differences, a battery of counterarguments
>> emerges: What about dogmatic and militant communists? What about how the
>> parties have differed through history? After all, the most canonical case
>> of ideologically driven science denial is probably the rejection of
>> genetics in the Soviet Union, where researchers disagreeing with the
>> anti-Mendelian scientist (and Stalin stooge) Trofim Lysenko were executed,
>> and genetics itself was denounced as a “bourgeois” science and officially
>> banned.
>> The upshot: All we can currently bank on is the fact that we all have
>> blinders in some situations. The question then becomes: What can be done to
>> counteract human nature itself?
>> *GIVEN THE POWER OF* our prior beliefs to skew how we respond to new
>> information, one thing is becoming clear: If you want someone to accept new
>> evidence, make sure to present it to them in a context that doesn’t trigger
>> a defensive, emotional reaction.
>> This theory is gaining traction in part because of Kahan’s work at Yale.
>> In one study<http://www.scribd.com/doc/3446682/The-Second-National-Risk-and-Culture-Study-Making-Sense-of-and-Making-Progress-In-The-American-Culture-War-of-Fact>,
>> he and his colleagues packaged the basic science of climate change into
>> fake newspaper articles bearing two very different headlines—”Scientific
>> Panel Recommends Anti-Pollution Solution to Global Warming” and “Scientific
>> Panel Recommends Nuclear Solution to Global Warming”—and then tested how
>> citizens with different values responded. Sure enough, the latter framing
>> made hierarchical individualists much more open to accepting the fact that
>> humans are causing global warming. Kahan infers that the effect occurred
>> because the science had been written into an alternative narrative that
>> appealed to their pro-industry worldview.
>> You can follow the logic to its conclusion: Conservatives are more likely
>> to embrace climate science if it comes to them via a business or religious
>> leader, who can set the issue in the context of different values than those
>> from which environmentalists or scientists often argue. Doing so is,
>> effectively, to signal a détente in what Kahan has called a “culture war of
>> fact.” In other words, paradoxically, you don’t lead with the facts in
>> order to convince. You lead with the values—so as to give the facts a
>> fighting chance.
>> *This **story*<http://www.motherjones.com/politics/2011/03/denial-science-chris-mooney>
>> * first appeared in **Mother Jones* <http://www.motherjones.com/>*
>>  magazine.*
>>
>> =======================================================
>>  List services made available by First Step Internet,
>>  serving the communities of the Palouse since 1994.
>>                http://www.fsr.net
>>           mailto:Vision2020 at moscow.com
>> =======================================================
>>
>
>
>
> --
> Art Deco (Wayne A. Fox)
> art.deco.studios at gmail.com
>
>
>
> =======================================================
>  List services made available by First Step Internet,
>  serving the communities of the Palouse since 1994.
>                http://www.fsr.net
>           mailto:Vision2020 at moscow.com
> =======================================================
>
-------------- next part --------------
An HTML attachment was scrubbed...
URL: <http://mailman.fsr.com/pipermail/vision2020/attachments/20130619/25046c09/attachment-0001.html>


More information about the Vision2020 mailing list